Just finished a stimulating, thought-provoking week at SRTL —Statistics Research Teaching and Learning conference–this year held in Two Harbors Minnesota, right on Lake Superior. SRTL gathers statistics education researchers, most of whom come with cognitive or educational psychology credentials, every two years. It’s more of a forum for thinking and collaborating than it is a platform for presenting findings, and this means there’s much lively, constructive discussion about works in progress.
I had meant to post my thoughts daily, but (a) the internet connection was unreliable and (b) there was just too much too digest. One recurring theme that really resonated with me was the ways students interact with technology when thinking about statistics. Much of the discussion centered on young learners, and most of the researchers – but not all – were in classrooms in which the students used TinkerPlots 2. Tinkerplots is a dynamic software system that lets kids build their own chance models. (It also lets them build their own graphics more-or-less from scratch.) They do this by either dropping “balls” into “urns” and labeling the balls with characteristics, or through spinners which allow them to shade different areas different colors. They can connect series of spinners and urns in order to create sequences of independent or dependent events, and can collect outcomes of their trials. Most importantly, they can carry out a large number of trials very quickly and graph the results.
What I found fascinating was the way in which students would come to judgements about situations, and then build a model that they thought would “prove” their point. After running some trials, when things didn’t go as expected, they would go back and assess their model. Sometimes they’d realize that they had made a mistake, and they’d fix it. Other times, they’d see there was no mistake, and then realize that they had been thinking about it wrong.Sometimes, they’d come up with explanations for why they had been thinking about it incorrectly.
Janet Ainley put it very succinctly. (More succinctly and precisely than my re-telling.) This technology imposes a sort of discipline on students' thinking. Using the technology is easy enough that they can be creative, but the technology is rigid enough that their mistakes are made apparent. This means that mistakes are cheap, and attempts to repair mistakes are easily made. And so the technology itself becomes a form of communication that forces students into a level of greater precision than they can put in words.
I suppose that mathematics plays the same role in that speaking with mathematics imposes great precision on the speaker. But that language takes time to learn, and few students reach a level of proficiency that allows them to use the language to construct new ideas. But Tinkerplots, and software like it, gives students the ability to use a language to express new ideas with very little expertise. It was impressive to see 15-year-olds build models that incorporated both deterministic trends and fairly sophisticated random variability. More impressive still, the students were able to use these models to solve problems. In fact, I’m not sure they really know they were building models at all, since their focus was on the problem solving.
Tinkerplots is aimed at a younger audience than the one I teach. But for me, the take-home message is to remember that statistical software isn’t simply a tool for calculation, but a tool for thinking.